File Download
  Links for fulltext
     (May Require Subscription)
Supplementary

Article: Ruminative and mood associations for age differences in social and directive reasons to think and talk about life experiences

TitleRuminative and mood associations for age differences in social and directive reasons to think and talk about life experiences
Authors
KeywordsDepression
Emotions
Memory
Elderly
Young adults
Issue Date2020
PublisherPublic Library of Science. The Journal's web site is located at http://www.plosone.org/home.action
Citation
PLoS One, 2020, v. 15, p. article no. e0235378 How to Cite?
AbstractReminiscing, or thinking and talking about our past experiences, can have mood enhancing effects. Rumination is implicated in reminiscence and yet has been shown to have negative effects on mood, with important differences between age groups. However, age differences in the effects of reminiscing on mood, and particularly the effects of rumination within reminiscence, are less explored. Two different age groups completed self-report measures of the positive directive (planning for present and future behaviors) and social (communication) uses of autobiographical memory, as well as maladaptive rumination and depression symptom severity. Young participants (Mean age: 21.82) ruminated more and reported greater frequency of using the directive and social functions of thinking and talking about past experiences than older adults (Mean age: 71.82). These reminiscence processes were also differentially associated with depression symptoms between age groups when tested in structural equation models. In older participants, but not young participants, the directive function was negatively associated with depression severity; in young participants, but not old participants, the social function was negatively associated with depression severity. Furthermore, although depressive and abstract rumination was directly positively related to depression scores in both samples, this association was inverted when the effect of rumination on depression was calculated through functions of reminiscence. The implications of these results for intervention development in older versus young adults, is discussed.
Persistent Identifierhttp://hdl.handle.net/10722/285007
ISSN
2021 Impact Factor: 3.752
2020 SCImago Journal Rankings: 0.990
PubMed Central ID
ISI Accession Number ID

 

DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.authorRicarte, JJ-
dc.contributor.authorRos, L-
dc.contributor.authorLatorre, JM-
dc.contributor.authorBarry, TJ-
dc.date.accessioned2020-08-07T09:05:32Z-
dc.date.available2020-08-07T09:05:32Z-
dc.date.issued2020-
dc.identifier.citationPLoS One, 2020, v. 15, p. article no. e0235378-
dc.identifier.issn1932-6203-
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10722/285007-
dc.description.abstractReminiscing, or thinking and talking about our past experiences, can have mood enhancing effects. Rumination is implicated in reminiscence and yet has been shown to have negative effects on mood, with important differences between age groups. However, age differences in the effects of reminiscing on mood, and particularly the effects of rumination within reminiscence, are less explored. Two different age groups completed self-report measures of the positive directive (planning for present and future behaviors) and social (communication) uses of autobiographical memory, as well as maladaptive rumination and depression symptom severity. Young participants (Mean age: 21.82) ruminated more and reported greater frequency of using the directive and social functions of thinking and talking about past experiences than older adults (Mean age: 71.82). These reminiscence processes were also differentially associated with depression symptoms between age groups when tested in structural equation models. In older participants, but not young participants, the directive function was negatively associated with depression severity; in young participants, but not old participants, the social function was negatively associated with depression severity. Furthermore, although depressive and abstract rumination was directly positively related to depression scores in both samples, this association was inverted when the effect of rumination on depression was calculated through functions of reminiscence. The implications of these results for intervention development in older versus young adults, is discussed.-
dc.languageeng-
dc.publisherPublic Library of Science. The Journal's web site is located at http://www.plosone.org/home.action-
dc.relation.ispartofPLoS ONE-
dc.rightsThis work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.-
dc.subjectDepression-
dc.subjectEmotions-
dc.subjectMemory-
dc.subjectElderly-
dc.subjectYoung adults-
dc.titleRuminative and mood associations for age differences in social and directive reasons to think and talk about life experiences-
dc.typeArticle-
dc.identifier.emailBarry, TJ: tjbarry@hku.hk-
dc.identifier.authorityBarry, TJ=rp02277-
dc.description.naturepublished_or_final_version-
dc.identifier.doi10.1371/journal.pone.0235378-
dc.identifier.pmid32673334-
dc.identifier.pmcidPMC7365414-
dc.identifier.scopuseid_2-s2.0-85088156225-
dc.identifier.hkuros312568-
dc.identifier.volume15-
dc.identifier.spagearticle no. e0235378-
dc.identifier.epagearticle no. e0235378-
dc.identifier.isiWOS:000553976700022-
dc.publisher.placeUnited States-
dc.identifier.issnl1932-6203-

Export via OAI-PMH Interface in XML Formats


OR


Export to Other Non-XML Formats