File Download
There are no files associated with this item.
Links for fulltext
(May Require Subscription)
- Publisher Website: 10.1007/s00221-007-1130-x
- Scopus: eid_2-s2.0-38649113621
- PMID: 17909769
- WOS: WOS:000252691000006
- Find via
Supplementary
-
Bookmarks:
- CiteULike: 1
- Citations:
- Appears in Collections:
Article: The effect of the “rod-and-frame” illusion on grip planning in a sequential object manipulation task
Title | The effect of the “rod-and-frame” illusion on grip planning in a sequential object manipulation task |
---|---|
Authors | |
Keywords | Biomedicine Neurosciences Neurology |
Issue Date | 2007 |
Publisher | Springer Berlin / Heidelberg |
Citation | Experimental Brain Research, 2007, v. 185, n. 1, p. 53-62 How to Cite? |
Persistent Identifier | http://hdl.handle.net/10722/147090 |
ISSN | 2023 Impact Factor: 1.7 2023 SCImago Journal Rankings: 0.613 |
ISI Accession Number ID | |
References | Aglioti S, DeSouza JFX, Goodale MA (1995) Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Curr Biol 5(6):679–685 doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(95)00133-3 Carey DP (2001) Do action systems resist visual illusions? Trends Cogn Sci 5(3):109–113 doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01592-8 Cohen RG, Rosenbaum DA (2004) Where grasps are made reveals how grasps are planned: generation and recall of motor plans. Exp Brain Res 157:486–495 doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-1862-9 Danckert JA, Sharif N, Haffenden AM, Schiff KC, Goodale MA (2002) A temporal analysis of grasping in the Ebbinghaus illusion: planning versus online control. Exp Brain Res 144:275–280 doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1073-1 DiLorenzo JR, Rock I (1982) The rod-and-frame effect as a function of the righting of the frame. J Exp Psychol Human 8(4):536–546 doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.8.4.536 Dyde RT, Milner AD (2002) Two illusions of perceived orientations: one fools all of the people some of the time; the other fools all of the people all of the time. Exp Brain Res 144:518–527 doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1065-1 Franz VH (2001) Action does not resist visual illusions. Trends Cogn Sci 5(11):457–459 doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01772-1 Gentilucci M, Negrotti A, Gangitano M (1997) Planning an action. Exp Brain Res 115:116–128 doi: 10.1007/PL00005671 Glover S (2002) Visual illusions affect planning but not control. Trends Cogn Sci 6(7):288–292 doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01920-4 Glover S, Dixon P (2002) Semantics affect the planning but not control of grasping. Exp Brain Res 146:383–387 doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1222-6 Glover S, Dixon P, Castiello U, Rushworth MFS (2005) Effects of an orientation illusion on motor performance and motor imagery. Exp Brain Res 166:17–22 doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2328-4 Goodale MA, Milner AD (2004) Plans for action. Behav Brain Sci 27(1):37–40 doi: 10.1017/S0140525X04340021 Haffenden AM, Goodale MA (1998) The effect of pictorial illusion on prehension and perception. J Cogn Neurosci 10(1):122–136 doi: 10.1162/089892998563824 Johnson-Frey SH, McCartney ME, Keen R (2004) Reaching beyond spatial perception: effects of intended future actions on visually guided prehension. Vis Cogn 11(2/3):371–399 doi: 10.1080/13506280344000329 Kelso JAS, Buchanan JJ, Murata T (1994) Multifunctionality and switching in the coordination dynamics of reaching and grasping. Hum Mov Sci 13:63–94 doi: 10.1016/0167-9457(94)90029-9 Mendoza JE, Elliott D, Meegan DV, Lyons JL, Welsh TN (2006) The effect of the Müller–Lyer Illusion on the planning and control of manual aiming movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32(2):413–422 doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.2.413 Meulenbroek RGJ, Rosenbaum DA, Jansen C, Vaughan J, Vogt S (2001) Multijoint grasping movements Simulated and observed effects of object location, object size, and initial aperture. Exp Brain Res 138:219–234 doi: 10.1007/s002210100690 Rosenbaum DA, Jorgensen MJ (1992) Planning macroscopic aspects of manual control. Hum Mov Sci 11:61–69 doi: 10.1016/0167-9457(92)90050-L Rosenbaum DA, Vaughan J, Barnes HJ, Jorgensen MJ (1992) Time course of movement planning: selection of handgrips for object manipulation. J Exp Psychol Learn 18(5):1058–1073 doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.18.5.1058 Rosenbaum DA, van Heugten CM, Caldwell GE (1996) From cognition to biomechanics and back: The en-state comfort effect and the middle-is-faster effect. Acta Psychol 94:59–85 doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(95)00062-3 Rosenbaum DA, Meulenbroek RJ, Vaughan J, Jansen C (2001) Posture-based motion planning: applications to grasping. Psychol Rev 108(4):709–734 doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.4.709 Short MW, Cauraugh JH (1999) Precision and the end-state comfort effect. Acta Psychol 100:243–252 doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(98)00020-1 Smeets JBJ, Brenner E, de Grave DDJ, Cuijpers RH (2002) Illusions in action: consequences of inconsistent processing of spatial attributes. Exp Brain Res 147:135–144 doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1185-7 Steenbergen B, Marteniuk RG, Kalbfleisch LE (1995). Achieving coordination in prehension: Joint freezing and postural contributions. J Motor Behav 27:333-348 doi: 10.1080/00222895.1995.9941722 Steenbergen B, Hulstijn W, Dortmans S (2000) Constraints on grip selection in cerebral palsy. Minimising discomfort Exp Brain Res 134:385–397 doi: 10.1007/s002210000458 Van Doorn H, Van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJP (2007) Grasping the Müller–Lyer illusion: the contributions of vision for perception in action. Neuropsychologia 45(8):1939–1947 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.008 |
DC Field | Value | Language |
---|---|---|
dc.contributor.author | Crajé, C | en_US |
dc.contributor.author | van der Kamp, J | en_US |
dc.contributor.author | Steenbergen, B | en_US |
dc.date.accessioned | 2012-05-25T07:50:35Z | - |
dc.date.available | 2012-05-25T07:50:35Z | - |
dc.date.issued | 2007 | en_US |
dc.identifier.citation | Experimental Brain Research, 2007, v. 185, n. 1, p. 53-62 | en_US |
dc.identifier.issn | 0014-4819 | en_US |
dc.identifier.uri | http://hdl.handle.net/10722/147090 | - |
dc.language | eng | en_US |
dc.publisher | Springer Berlin / Heidelberg | en_US |
dc.relation.ispartof | Experimental Brain Research | en_US |
dc.subject | Biomedicine | en_US |
dc.subject | Neurosciences | en_US |
dc.subject | Neurology | en_US |
dc.title | The effect of the “rod-and-frame” illusion on grip planning in a sequential object manipulation task | en_US |
dc.type | Article | en_US |
dc.identifier.openurl | http://www.springerlink.com/link-out/?id=2104&code=T44632R6X61Q4869&MUD=MP | en_US |
dc.description.nature | link_to_OA_fulltext | en_US |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.1007/s00221-007-1130-x | en_US |
dc.identifier.pmid | 17909769 | - |
dc.identifier.scopus | eid_2-s2.0-38649113621 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Aglioti S, DeSouza JFX, Goodale MA (1995) Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Curr Biol 5(6):679–685 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(95)00133-3 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Carey DP (2001) Do action systems resist visual illusions? Trends Cogn Sci 5(3):109–113 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01592-8 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Cohen RG, Rosenbaum DA (2004) Where grasps are made reveals how grasps are planned: generation and recall of motor plans. Exp Brain Res 157:486–495 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-1862-9 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Danckert JA, Sharif N, Haffenden AM, Schiff KC, Goodale MA (2002) A temporal analysis of grasping in the Ebbinghaus illusion: planning versus online control. Exp Brain Res 144:275–280 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1073-1 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | DiLorenzo JR, Rock I (1982) The rod-and-frame effect as a function of the righting of the frame. J Exp Psychol Human 8(4):536–546 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.8.4.536 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Dyde RT, Milner AD (2002) Two illusions of perceived orientations: one fools all of the people some of the time; the other fools all of the people all of the time. Exp Brain Res 144:518–527 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1065-1 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Franz VH (2001) Action does not resist visual illusions. Trends Cogn Sci 5(11):457–459 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01772-1 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Gentilucci M, Negrotti A, Gangitano M (1997) Planning an action. Exp Brain Res 115:116–128 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1007/PL00005671 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Glover S, Dixon P (2001a) The role of vision in the on-line correction of illusion effects on action. Can J Exp Psychol 55(2):96–103 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Glover SR, Dixon P (2001b) Dynamic illusion effects in a reaching task: evidence for separate visual representations in the planning and control of reaching. J Exp Psychol Human 27(3):560–572 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Glover S (2002) Visual illusions affect planning but not control. Trends Cogn Sci 6(7):288–292 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01920-4 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Glover S, Dixon P (2002) Semantics affect the planning but not control of grasping. Exp Brain Res 146:383–387 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1222-6 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Glover S (2004) Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action. Behav Brain Sci 27:3–78 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Glover S, Dixon P, Castiello U, Rushworth MFS (2005) Effects of an orientation illusion on motor performance and motor imagery. Exp Brain Res 166:17–22 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2328-4 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Goodale MA, Milner AD (1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Cogn Sci 15(1):20–25 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Goodale MA, Milner AD (2004) Plans for action. Behav Brain Sci 27(1):37–40 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1017/S0140525X04340021 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Haffenden AM, Goodale MA (1998) The effect of pictorial illusion on prehension and perception. J Cogn Neurosci 10(1):122–136 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1162/089892998563824 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Jackson SR, Shaw A (2000) The ponzo illusion affects grip-force but not grip-aperture scaling during prehension movements. J Exp Psychol Human 26:418–423 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Johnson-Frey SH, McCartney ME, Keen R (2004) Reaching beyond spatial perception: effects of intended future actions on visually guided prehension. Vis Cogn 11(2/3):371–399 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1080/13506280344000329 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Kelso JAS, Buchanan JJ, Murata T (1994) Multifunctionality and switching in the coordination dynamics of reaching and grasping. Hum Mov Sci 13:63–94 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/0167-9457(94)90029-9 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Marteniuk RG, MacKenzie CL, Jeannerod M (1987) Constraints on Human arm movement trajectories. Can J Psychol 41:365–378 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Mendoza JE, Elliott D, Meegan DV, Lyons JL, Welsh TN (2006) The effect of the Müller–Lyer Illusion on the planning and control of manual aiming movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32(2):413–422 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.2.413 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Meulenbroek RGJ, Rosenbaum DA, Jansen C, Vaughan J, Vogt S (2001) Multijoint grasping movements Simulated and observed effects of object location, object size, and initial aperture. Exp Brain Res 138:219–234 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1007/s002210100690 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Rosenbaum DA, Jorgensen MJ (1992) Planning macroscopic aspects of manual control. Hum Mov Sci 11:61–69 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/0167-9457(92)90050-L | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Rosenbaum DA, Vaughan J, Barnes HJ, Jorgensen MJ (1992) Time course of movement planning: selection of handgrips for object manipulation. J Exp Psychol Learn 18(5):1058–1073 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.18.5.1058 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Rosenbaum DA, van Heugten CM, Caldwell GE (1996) From cognition to biomechanics and back: The en-state comfort effect and the middle-is-faster effect. Acta Psychol 94:59–85 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(95)00062-3 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Rosenbaum DA, Meulenbroek RJ, Vaughan J, Jansen C (2001) Posture-based motion planning: applications to grasping. Psychol Rev 108(4):709–734 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.4.709 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Short MW, Cauraugh JH (1999) Precision and the end-state comfort effect. Acta Psychol 100:243–252 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(98)00020-1 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Smeets JBJ, Brenner E (1995) Perception and action are based on the same visual information: distinction between position and velocity. J Exp Psychol Hum 21(1):19–31 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Smeets JBJ, Brenner E, de Grave DDJ, Cuijpers RH (2002) Illusions in action: consequences of inconsistent processing of spatial attributes. Exp Brain Res 147:135–144 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1185-7 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Steenbergen B, Marteniuk RG, Kalbfleisch LE (1995). Achieving coordination in prehension: Joint freezing and postural contributions. J Motor Behav 27:333-348 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1080/00222895.1995.9941722 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Steenbergen B, Hulstijn W, Dortmans S (2000) Constraints on grip selection in cerebral palsy. Minimising discomfort Exp Brain Res 134:385–397 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1007/s002210000458 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Van Doorn H, Van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJP (2007) Grasping the Müller–Lyer illusion: the contributions of vision for perception in action. Neuropsychologia 45(8):1939–1947 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.008 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Beh HC, Wenderoth PM (1971) The effect of variation of frame shape on the angular function of the rod-and-frame illusion. Percept Psychophys 11:35–37 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Dijkerman HC, Schindler RD, McIntosh RD, Nijboer TCW, Milner AD (2003) Choosing between alternative wrist postures: action planning needs perception. Congress of European Neuropsychological Societies, Modena, Italy | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Franz VH, Scharnowski F, Gegenfurntner KR (2005) Illusion effects on grasping are temporally constant not dynamic. J Exp Psychol Human 313(6):1359–1378 | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Milner AD, Goodale MA (1995) The visual brain in action. Oxford University Press, New York | en_US |
dc.relation.references | Rosenbaum DA, Vaughan J, Jorgensen MJ, Barnes HJ, Steward E (1993) Plans for object manipulation. In: Myer DE, Kornblum S (eds) Attention and performance, vol 14. MIT, Cambridge, pp 803–820 | en_US |
dc.identifier.volume | 185 | en_US |
dc.identifier.issue | 1 | en_US |
dc.identifier.spage | 53 | en_US |
dc.identifier.epage | 62 | en_US |
dc.identifier.eissn | 1432-1106 | en_US |
dc.identifier.isi | WOS:000252691000006 | - |
dc.description.other | Springer Open Choice, 25 May 2012 | en_US |
dc.identifier.citeulike | 8413572 | - |
dc.identifier.issnl | 0014-4819 | - |