File Download
There are no files associated with this item.
Links for fulltext
(May Require Subscription)
- Publisher Website: 10.1007/s10548-015-0438-2
- Scopus: eid_2-s2.0-84953367093
- PMID: 26012382
- WOS: WOS:000368150700007
- Find via
Supplementary
- Citations:
- Appears in Collections:
Article: Dissociating the Influence of Affective Word Content and Cognitive Processing Demands on the Late Positive Potential
Title | Dissociating the Influence of Affective Word Content and Cognitive Processing Demands on the Late Positive Potential |
---|---|
Authors | |
Keywords | Permutation test Emotion Event-related potential (ERP) Late positive potential (LPP) P300 Residue iteration decomposition (RIDE) |
Issue Date | 2016 |
Citation | Brain Topography, 2016, v. 29, n. 1, p. 82-93 How to Cite? |
Abstract | © 2015, Springer Science+Business Media New York. The late positive potential (LPP) elicited by affective stimuli in the event-related brain potential (ERP) is often assumed to be a member of the P3 family. The present study addresses the relationship of the LPP to the classic P3b in a published data set, using a non-parametric permutation test for topographical comparisons, and residue iteration decomposition to assess the temporal features of the LPP and the P3b by decomposing the ERP into several component clusters according to their latency variability. The experiment orthogonally manipulated arousal and valence of words, which were either read or judged for lexicality. High-arousing and positive valenced words induced a larger LPP than low-arousing and negative valenced words, respectively, and t he LDT elicited a larger P3b than reading. The experimental manipulation of arousal, valence, and task yielded main effects without any interactions on ERP amplitude in the LPP/P3b time range. The arousal and valence effects partially differed from the task effect in scalp topography; in addition, whereas the late positive component elicited by affective stimuli, defined as LPP, was stimulus-locked, the late positive component elicited by task demand, defined as P3b, was mainly latency-variable. Therefore LPP and P3b manifest different subcomponents. |
Persistent Identifier | http://hdl.handle.net/10722/246823 |
ISSN | 2023 Impact Factor: 2.3 2023 SCImago Journal Rankings: 0.863 |
ISI Accession Number ID |
DC Field | Value | Language |
---|---|---|
dc.contributor.author | Nowparast Rostami, Hadiseh | - |
dc.contributor.author | Ouyang, Guang | - |
dc.contributor.author | Bayer, Mareike | - |
dc.contributor.author | Schacht, Annekathrin | - |
dc.contributor.author | Zhou, Changsong | - |
dc.contributor.author | Sommer, Werner | - |
dc.date.accessioned | 2017-09-26T04:28:05Z | - |
dc.date.available | 2017-09-26T04:28:05Z | - |
dc.date.issued | 2016 | - |
dc.identifier.citation | Brain Topography, 2016, v. 29, n. 1, p. 82-93 | - |
dc.identifier.issn | 0896-0267 | - |
dc.identifier.uri | http://hdl.handle.net/10722/246823 | - |
dc.description.abstract | © 2015, Springer Science+Business Media New York. The late positive potential (LPP) elicited by affective stimuli in the event-related brain potential (ERP) is often assumed to be a member of the P3 family. The present study addresses the relationship of the LPP to the classic P3b in a published data set, using a non-parametric permutation test for topographical comparisons, and residue iteration decomposition to assess the temporal features of the LPP and the P3b by decomposing the ERP into several component clusters according to their latency variability. The experiment orthogonally manipulated arousal and valence of words, which were either read or judged for lexicality. High-arousing and positive valenced words induced a larger LPP than low-arousing and negative valenced words, respectively, and t he LDT elicited a larger P3b than reading. The experimental manipulation of arousal, valence, and task yielded main effects without any interactions on ERP amplitude in the LPP/P3b time range. The arousal and valence effects partially differed from the task effect in scalp topography; in addition, whereas the late positive component elicited by affective stimuli, defined as LPP, was stimulus-locked, the late positive component elicited by task demand, defined as P3b, was mainly latency-variable. Therefore LPP and P3b manifest different subcomponents. | - |
dc.language | eng | - |
dc.relation.ispartof | Brain Topography | - |
dc.subject | Permutation test | - |
dc.subject | Emotion | - |
dc.subject | Event-related potential (ERP) | - |
dc.subject | Late positive potential (LPP) | - |
dc.subject | P300 | - |
dc.subject | Residue iteration decomposition (RIDE) | - |
dc.title | Dissociating the Influence of Affective Word Content and Cognitive Processing Demands on the Late Positive Potential | - |
dc.type | Article | - |
dc.description.nature | link_to_subscribed_fulltext | - |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.1007/s10548-015-0438-2 | - |
dc.identifier.pmid | 26012382 | - |
dc.identifier.scopus | eid_2-s2.0-84953367093 | - |
dc.identifier.volume | 29 | - |
dc.identifier.issue | 1 | - |
dc.identifier.spage | 82 | - |
dc.identifier.epage | 93 | - |
dc.identifier.eissn | 1573-6792 | - |
dc.identifier.isi | WOS:000368150700007 | - |
dc.identifier.issnl | 0896-0267 | - |